Thinking Outside the Box: A Misguided Idea | Psychology Today Thinking Outside the Box: A Misguided Idea | Psychology Today

Dating rules letmewatchthis, browse torrents

In other words, the difference could easily be due to what statisticians call sampling error. In fact, only a meager 25 percent did. Or so their consultants would have them believe. They are much more common than you probably think. If you have tried solving this puzzle, you can confirm that your first attempts usually involve sketching lines inside the imaginary square.

Would you like to guess the percentage of the participants in the second group who solved the puzzle correctly? It was an appealing and apparently convincing message. In the s, however, very few were even aware of its existence, even though it had been around for almost a century.

Because the solution is, in hindsight, deceptively simple, clients tended to admit they should have thought of it themselves.

1337x Links

Guilford was one of the first academic researchers who dared to conduct a study of creativity. Solving this problem requires people to literally think outside the box.

No one, that is, before two different research teams —Clarke Burnham with Kenneth Davis, and Joseph Alba with Robert Weisberg—ran another experiment using the same puzzle but a different research procedure.

The correct solution, however, requires you to draw lines that extend beyond the area defined by the dots. Today many people are familiar with this puzzle and its solution. Management consultants in the s and s even used this puzzle when making sales pitches to prospective clients.

The second group was told that the solution required the lines to be drawn outside the imaginary box bordering the dot array. Only 20 percent managed to break out of the illusory confinement and continue their lines in the white space surrounding the dots.

Free brazilian online dating sites

In the early s, a psychologist named J. That this advice is useless when actually trying to solve a problem involving a real box should effectively have killed off the much widely disseminated—and therefore, much more dangerous—metaphor that out-of-the-box thinking spurs creativity. SHARE Although studying creativity is considered a legitimate scientific discipline nowadays, it is still a very young one.

That is, direct and explicit instructions to think outside the box did not help. Speakers, trainers, training program developers, organizational consultants, and university professors all had much to say about the vast benefits of outside-the-box thinking.

There seemed to be no end to the insights that could be offered under the banner of thinking outside the box.

The idea went viral via s-era media and word of mouth, of course. Most people assume that 60 percent to 90 percent of the group given the clue would solve the puzzle easily. But you will find numerous situations where a creative breakthrough is staring you in the face. He challenged research subjects to connect all nine dots using just four straight lines without lifting their pencils from the page.

The symmetry, the beautiful simplicity of the solution, and the fact that 80 percent of the participants were effectively blinded by the boundaries of the square led Guilford and the readers of his books to leap to the sweeping conclusion that creativity requires you to go outside the box.

Both teams followed the same protocol of dividing participants into two groups. Overnight, it seemed that creativity gurus everywhere were teaching managers how to think outside the box. Indeed, the concept enjoyed such strong popularity and intuitive appeal that no one bothered to check the facts.

After all, with one simple yet brilliant experiment, researchers had proven that the conceptual link between thinking outside the box and creativity was a myth.